The Safe Gas That Helps Stop Distressing Memories From Getting Stuck In The Mind

The experience of distressing, intrusive memories was reduced by over a half.

The experience of distressing, intrusive memories was reduced by over a half.

Inhaling laughing gas after a traumatic event reduces the later intrusion of distressing memories, new research finds.

People who inhaled nitrous oxide experienced fewer intrusive thoughts a day later, the study found.

Dr Ravi Das, the study’s first author, explained:

“The day after they saw the film, the number of intrusions experienced by the group who received nitrous oxide fell by over a half.

By contrast, the decline in intrusions was much slower in the group who received air, where there was not a significant drop in intrusions until the fourth day.

We think that this is because nitrous oxide disrupts a process that helps permanent memories to form.”

The study involved participants watching a distressing video clip.

Afterwards, half were given air, the other half a mixture containing the laughing gas.

Dr Das said:

“On any given day your brain will be exposed to a huge amount of information, some important, but most trivial.

If information is ‘important’ enough to remember, for instance because it produces a strong emotional response, it is ‘tagged’ for storage.

The brain requires N-Methyl D-Aspartate (NMDA) receptors to tag information during the day, which is then filed for long-term storage while we sleep.

We know that nitrous oxide blocks NMDA receptors, so could interfere with tagging.

This might explain why the nitrous oxide group seemed to have weaker memories of the film the day after they watched it.”

Dr Sunjeev Kamboj, one of the study’s co-authors, said:

“Nitrous oxide is routinely used as a painkiller by paramedics and in A&E departments because it is safe and easy to administer.

Many people who end up in an ambulance will have undergone some form of psychological trauma, and our study suggests that the nitrous oxide is likely to be having some effect on how their brain processes it.

However, whether it helps to prevent symptoms of PTSD or makes them more likely may depend on how dissociated patients feel before they receive it.

Further research is now needed to determine whether dissociation similarly affects the response of trauma victims who receive nitrous oxide or other painkillers such as ketamine.

It is worth noting that our volunteers were given nitrous oxide continuously for half an hour, whereas the amount that hospital patients receive will vary significantly.

Some may only receive small doses in the ambulance whereas others could be breathing it on-and-off for hours in a hospital bed.

Any effects, positive or negative, are also likely to vary depending on the dose.

The amount of nitrous oxide used recreationally in a balloon, for example, would likely be too small to have a noticeable effect on memory formation.”

The study was published in the journal Psychological Medicine (Das et al., 2016).

Sad man image from Shutterstock

Memory And Attention Work Best At Certain Times Of The Year, Study Finds

New research suggests people’s cognitive abilities are affected by the seasons.

New research suggests people’s cognitive abilities are affected by the seasons.

Brain scans show that people’s short-term memory is generally best in fall (autumn) and worst in spring.

In mid-December, though, people’s attention skills are at their lowest ebb.

Attention doesn’t come up to full strength until June.

Dr Gilles Vandewalle, one of the study’s authors, said that the causes are likely down to multiple factors, but:

“…we may be tuned to lower brain activity in winter, and that could cause changes in brain activity.

But in modern society we are similarly active throughout the year.”

It could also be that people who experience seasonally affected disorder may be particularly vulnerable to changes in cognition over the year.

Brain Scans Reveal Why Up To One in Six People Get SAD in Winter

The new research had 28 men and women visit the lab at different times of the year to take various tests.

Both brain scans and behavioural tests were used.

Only the brain scans showed the differences, not the behavioural tests.

The study was published in the journal PNAS (Meyer et al., 2016).

Image credit: Noukka Signe

Learning and Memory Skills Can Recover From The Diet That Damages Them

Certain diets can damage learning and memory, but the brain can recover, a new study finds.

Certain diets can damage learning and memory, but the brain can recover, a new study finds.

High-fat diets have long been linked to all kinds of psychological problems, including depression, anxiety and learning and memory deficits.

Now, research on mice has found that memory and learning problems disappear over time, even while maintaining the high-fat diet.

In the study, some of the mice were fed a high-fat diet.

Compared with another group fed a normal diet, after three months they showed severe problems with spatial memory.

But, after 12 months, their brains seemed to have adjusted to the high-fat diet.

High-Fat Diet May Disturb a Range of Thoughts And Feelings

Ms Erica Underwood, one of the study’s author, said:

“Other factors behind the impairment must be recovering in the long term.

That’s the beautiful thing about the brain.

It can often recover or find alternate routes to allow normal function in spite of impairment.”

While the mice’s brains coped with the diet, their bodies fared less well.

The high-fat diet caused obesity and the males developed the symptoms of type-2 diabetes.

Professor Lucien Thompson, who co-authored the study, said:

“We saw that the males’ brains, as well as bodies, became insulin-insensitive.

They were producing more insulin because of the fatty diet, but not enough to cover blood glucose surges after an oral dose of glucose.

And while insulin normally stimulates hippocampal neurons, high-fat diet males stopped responding to insulin.”

Ms Underwood concluded:

“For the first time in human history we have large numbers of toddlers who are severely overweight.

It’s unprecedented.

With this new type of physiology we’re seeing, we don’t really know what’s going to happen to these children 20 or 50 years from now.”

The study was published in the journal Neural Plasticity (Underwood & Thompson, 2015).

I am busy image from Shutterstock

Hot Chocolate Could Help Boost Memory And Thinking Skills

60 people were given tests of memory and thinking skills after drinking this for 30 days.

60 people were given tests of memory and thinking skills after drinking this for 30 days.

Two cups of hot chocolate a day could help keep the brain healthy, a recent study finds.

The research involved 60 people whose average age was 73.

They were given tests of memory and thinking skills and the blood flow in their brains was measured.

Dr Farzaneh A. Sorond, the study’s first author, said:

“We’re learning more about blood flow in the brain and its effect on thinking skills.

As different areas of the brain need more energy to complete their tasks, they also need greater blood flow.

This relationship, called neurovascular coupling, may play an important role in diseases such as Alzheimer’s.”

Half the people in the study were given hot cocoa rich in an antioxidant called flavanol.

The other half received flavanol-poor cocoa.

Both groups continued to have hot cocoa every day for two a month.

The results showed that people who had impaired blood flow in the brain improved after drinking the flavanol-rich cocoa.

People with impaired blood flow also improved on tests of memory and thinking skills.

There was no improvement for people with normal blood flow.

Dr Paul B. Rosenberg, who wrote an editorial accompanying the study, said:

“More work is needed to prove a link between cocoa, blood flow problems and cognitive decline.

But this is an important first step that could guide future studies.”

The study was published in the journal Neurology (Sorond et al., 2013).

Brain image from Shutterstock

How The Brain Can Play Out Memories And Future Events In Fast Forward

In a few seconds our brains can play out events that would take hours or days at real time.

In a few seconds our brains can play out events that would take hours or days at real time.

The imagination has an amazing ability to compress time.

In a few seconds our brains can play out events that would take hours or days at real time.

It’s an ability we take for granted and one we need to help plan for the future.

The key to how we are able to do this is a type of brain wave which is separate to the one we use for remembering real-time experiences.

Brain cells use a series of different electrical frequencies to communicate with each other.

It’s like the way radio waves are used to communicate over long distances.

One of these frequencies, however, allows the brain to play back memories or plan future events in fast forward.

Dr Laura Colgin, one of the study’s authors, explained:

“The reason we’re excited about it is that we think this mechanism can help explain how you can imagine a sequence of events you’re about to do in a time-compressed manner.

You can plan out those events and think about the sequences of actions you’ll do.

And all of that happens on a faster time scale when you’re imagining it than when you actually go and do those things.”

It’s known that fast gamma rhythms are used to encode memories happening right now, in real time.

The researchers discovered that slow gamma rhythms are used to retrieve memories and plan for the future in fast forward.

Naturally, the extra speed causes the memories and plans to lose resolution.

It’s the same as when you compress a file on the computer and it loses some information along the way.

The findings could also have implications for mental illness, Dr Colgin thinks:

“Maybe they are transmitting their own imagined thoughts on the wrong frequency, the one usually reserved for things that are really happening.

That could have terrible consequences.”

The study was published in the journal Neuron (Zheng et al., 2016).

Brain image from Shutterstock

Eating More Vegetables Linked To 40% Better Memory

Study of 3,718 older people found that, cognitively, some people’s brains were 5 years younger.

Study of 3,718 older people found that, cognitively, some people’s brains were 5 years younger.

Eating vegetables — but not fruit — helps preserve memory, research finds.

The study of 3,718 people over 65 living in Chicago asked how often people ate particular foods and administered cognitive tests.

Professor Martha Clare Morris, who led the study, explained the results:

“Compared to people who consumed less than one serving of vegetables a day, people who ate at least 2.8 servings of vegetables a day saw their rate of cognitive change slow by roughly 40 percent.

This decrease is equivalent to about 5 years of younger age.”

Green leafy vegetables showed the strongest association with a better memory.

Older people in the study got the greatest benefit from eating more vegetables.

More than two servings of vegetables per day was linked to the slowest rates of cognitive decline in older people.

Professor Morris was surprised to find there was no link between fruit consumption and a preserved memory:

“This was unanticipated and raises several questions.

It may be due to vegetables containing high amounts of vitamin E, which helps lowers the risk of cognitive decline.

Vegetables, but not fruits, are also typically consumed with added fats such as salad dressings, and fats increase the absorption of vitamin E.

Further study is required to understand why fruit is not associated with cognitive change.”

The study was published in the journal Neurology (Morris et al., 2006).

Vegetable brain image from Shutterstock

Late-Night Snacking Is A Widespread Habit That May Damage Memory

Learning and memory could be damaged by this common habit.

Learning and memory could be damaged by this common habit.

Habitual late-night snacking could damage a type of memory and learning, a new study suggests.

Eating meals at odd hours has already been linked to metabolic problems.

This, though, is some of the first evidence of potential psychological effects.

Dr Dawn Loh, the study’s first author, said:

“We have provided the first evidence that taking regular meals at the wrong time of day has far-reaching effects for learning and memory.

Since many people find themselves working or playing during times when they’d normally be asleep, it is important to know that this could dull some of the functions of the brain.”

The study, carried out on mice, tested their ability to recognise novel objects.

Those fed when they usually slept showed worse memory performance.

The late-night-snacking-mice also had problems filing the objects away in long-term memory.

Professor Christopher Colwell, one of the study’s author, said:

“Modern schedules can lead us to eat around the clock so it is important to understand how the timing of food can impact cogitation.

For the first time, we have shown that simply adjusting the time when food is made available alters the molecular clock in the hippocampus and can alter the cognitive performance of mice.”

The mice that snacked at night also had worse sleep.

Their sleep was more fragmented and they require more naps during the day.

Researchers will need to carry out tests on human subjects to see if the effect is the same.

However, we already know from other studies that shift-workers under-perform on cognitive tests.

We also know that eating less at night may help to reduce the mental problems caused by lack of sleep.

The study was published in the journal eLife (Loh et al., 2015).

Brain image from Shutterstock

The Totally Unexpected Key To Learning

One key to learning is the exact opposite of what you’d expect.

One key to learning is the exact opposite of what you’d expect.

Forgetting is a normal and necessary part of learning, a new study finds.

The instability of memory is the key to how we transfer skills and experiences to new situations, researchers have found.

Professor Edwin Robertson, from the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, said:

“Our work shows that an unstable memory is a key component of the mechanism for learning transfer.

An unstable memory prevents learning from being rigidly linked to one task; instead, it allows learning to be applied flexibly.

In this study we tested the link between a memory being unstable and the transfer of learning to a different type of memory task.

We measured how learning in one task transferred to and thus improved learning in a subsequent task.

There was transfer from a motor skill to a word list task and, vice versa, from a word list to a motor skill task.

What was transferred was a high-level relationship between elements, rather than knowledge of the individual elements themselves.”

For the research, people were given the two tests 12 hours apart.

The results showed that learning between the two different tasks was only transferred when memory was unstable.

Professor Robertson said:

“Stabilised memories consistently prevented transfer to the subsequent memory task.

This suggests that the transfer of learning across diverse tasks is due to a ‘high-level representation’ that can only be formed when a memory is unstable.

Our work has identified an important function of memory instability.

An unstable memory provides a window of opportunity for communication between memories, leading to the construction of a high-level or abstract memory representation, which allows the transfer of knowledge between memory tasks.

An unstable memory is in a privileged state: only when unstable can a memory communicate with and transfer knowledge to affect the acquisition of a subsequent memory.”

The study was published in the journal Current Biology (Mosha & Robertson, 2015).

How The Brain Forgets Things To Conserve Energy

A fascinating new explanation of why our brains forget some things we’ve learned.

A fascinating new explanation of why our brains forget some things we’ve learned.

The brain may forget in order to save energy, a new study suggests.

So, our brains contain mechanisms that help us erase unnecessary learning.

Now scientists have uncovered how this may happen at the cellular level.

The results come from a strange finding about how we learn.

You may know the story of Pavlov’s dogs, who were taught to salivate at the ringing of a bell because they associated it with being fed.

Similarly, both humans and animals can learn to link a certain tone with a puff of air to the eyes.

This, of course, causes people to blink — it’s an automatic response.

However, you can remove the actual puff of air to the eyes and people will blink at just the sound of the tone.

They have learned to link the tone to the puff of air so they blink — even when there’s no puff.

Here’s the kink in the story, though.

When you add the puff of air back in, paradoxically people’s learning gets worse.

It’s like the extra stimulus is actually causing forgetting.

Professor Germund Hesslow, who led the new research said:

“Two stimuli therfore achieve worse results than just one.

It seems contrary to common sense, but we believe that the reason for it is that the brain wants to save energy.”

What seems to be happening is that when we’ve successfully learned a link, a neuronal braking mechanism activates.

Professor Hesslow continued:

“You could say that the part of the brain that learned the association (a part of the brain called the cerebellum) is telling its ‘teacher’: ‘I know this now, please be quiet’.

When the brain has learnt two associations, the brake becomes much more powerful.

That is why it results in forgetting, usually only temporarily, however.”

The new research describes how the nerve cells learn and forget.

Professor Hesslow said:

“Obviously, it should be important for teachers to know the mechanisms by which the brain erases the things it considers unnecessary.

You do not want to accidentally activate these mechanisms.”

The study was published in the journal PNAS (Rasmussen et al., 2015).

Confused image from Shutterstock

Max Memory Boost By Combining Two Easy Methods

Even three months later those who combined both techniques had the best recall.

Even three months later those who combined both techniques had the best recall, study finds.

Rewards combined with naps can help you learn, a new study finds.

When memories are linked to rewards, researchers found they stick better in the brain.

Adding a short nap afterwards can provide an extra boost.

Dr Kinga Igloi, who led the research, said:

“Rewards may act as a kind of tag, sealing information in the brain during learning.

During sleep, that information is favourably consolidated over information associated with a low reward and is transferred to areas of the brain associated with long-term memory.

Our findings are relevant for understanding the devastating effects that lack of sleep can have on achievement.”

In the study people were given pairs of pictures to try and remember.

Some took naps after learning, while others did not.

Sometimes participants were given small rewards after learning as well.

The study revealed that people remembered the pairs of pictures better when they received a reward.

But it was the group that slept after learning that performed the best overall.

Brain scans also showed that those who slept had higher activity in the hippocampus, an area vital to forming new memories.

People were asked back into the lab three months later for a surprise test.

Even three months later the group who were rewarded and slept after learning had the best recall.

Dr Igloi said:

“We already knew that sleep helps strengthens memories, but we now also know that it helps us select and retain those that have a rewarding value.

It makes adaptive sense that the consolidation of memory should work to prioritise information that is critical to our success and survival.”

The study was published in the journal eLife (Igloi et al., 2015).

Brain image from Shutterstock

Get free email updates

Join the free PsyBlog mailing list. No spam, ever.