Cocktail Party Effect In Psychology: Definition & Example

The definition of the cocktail party effect in psychology  is when we tune into one voice from many conversations going on in a noisy room.

The definition of the cocktail party effect in psychology  is when we tune into one voice from many conversations going on in a noisy room.

For psychologists the ‘cocktail party effect’ or phenomenon is our impressive and under-appreciated ability to tune our attention to just one voice from a multitude.

For example, at a party, when bored with our current conversational partner — and for the compulsive eavesdropper — we can allow our aural attention to wander around the room.

Perhaps only the most recidivist eavesdroppers are aware how special the cocktail party effect is.

But even they might be surprised — and worried — by just how much we can miss in the voices we decide to tune out.

What is the cocktail party effect?

The cocktail party effect or phenomenon — our ability to separate one conversation from another — is beautifully demonstrated in a classic study carried out by Colin Cherry (Cherry, 1953).

Cherry used the simple method of playing back two different messages at the same time to people, under a variety of conditions.

In doing so he discovered just how good we are at filtering what we hear, which is how we overcome the cocktail party problem.

To accomplish this task, Cherry reports, participants had to close their eyes and concentrate hard.

In the first set of experiments on the cocktail party effect he played back two different messages voiced by the same person through both ears of a pair of headphones and asked participants to ‘shadow’ one of the two messages they were hearing by speaking it out loud, and later by writing it down.

When doing this they could, with effort, and while hearing the clips over and over again, separate one of the messages from the other.

With the two voice presented together, as though the same person were standing in front of you saying two completely different things at the same time, this task appears to be very hard, but still possible.

Pushing participants further Cherry found he could confuse listeners, but only by having both messages consist entirely of nonsensical platitudes.

Only then were participants unable to pick apart one message from the other.

This is not a wholly satisfying demonstration of the cocktail party effect.

An example of how the cocktail party effect works

The real surprise, though, came in the second set of experiments on the cocktail party effect or phenomenon.

For these Cherry fed one message to the left ear and one to the right ear — and once again both messages were voiced by the same speaker.

Suddenly participants found the task incredibly easy.

Indeed many were surprised how easily and accurately they could tune in to either one of the messages, and even shift their attention back and forth between the two.

No longer did they have to close their eyes and furrow their brows – this was much easier.

What participants were experiencing here seems much closer to most people’s experience of the cocktail party phenomenon.

At a party people are arrayed all around us and their conversations come from various different directions.

We seem to be able to use this information, which is key to the cocktail party effect, to reject all but the one in which we are interested.

Ignoring rejected speech

Although we are fantastically good at tuning in to one conversation over all the others, we seem to absorb very little information from the conversations we reject.

This is the flipside of the cocktail party effect and where it can get embarrassing.

Cherry’s experiments on the cocktail party effect revealed that people picked up surprisingly little information presented to the other, ‘rejected ear’, often failing to notice blatant changes to the unattended message.

When asked afterwards, participants:

  • could not identify a single phrase from the speech presented to the rejected ear.
  • weren’t sure the language in the rejected ear was even English.
  • failed to notice when it changed to German.
  • mostly didn’t notice when the speech to the rejected ear was being played backwards (though some did report that it sounded a bit strange).

Across all the different conditions in these cocktail party effect studies, there were only two aspects of the speech to the rejected ear the participants could reliably identify.

The first was that it was speech compared to a tone, the second was when the speaker suddenly changed from male to female.

Missed your own name

This research on the cocktail party effect doesn’t bode at all well for people with a habit of tuning out of conversations when they  lose interest (you know who you are!).

If you really are listening to someone else it’s likely you won’t hear a word of what’s being said to you directly.

One study has found that two-thirds of people don’t even notice when their own name is slipped into the unattended speech, while those who do notice are likely to be of the extremely distractable variety (Conway et al., 2001).

That demonstrates the power of the cocktail party effect.

.

Why Gut-Decisions Beat Agonising Over Business Data

The unconscious, or gut instinct, can do just as well as a conscious, deliberate decision in the business environment.

The unconscious, or gut instinct, can do just as well as a conscious, deliberate decision in the business environment.

Instinctive, gut decisions may be just as good as those based on data, a study suggests.

Managers who rely on gut instinct to make decisions about new projects are just as likely to be right as those relying on the data, the research found.

However, relying on gut instinct is much faster, as data analysis typically takes a long time.

Think or blink?

Malcolm Gladwell wrote a well-known book called ‘Blink‘ about the power of the unconscious to make complex decisions in the blink of an eye.

However, since then studies have failed to back up the idea that the unconscious can outperform the conscious mind.

Still, there is evidence that the unconscious, or gut instinct, can do just as well as a conscious, deliberate decision.

And as this study underlines, sometimes gut decisions have other benefits, such as speed and requiring fewer resources.

The study comes at a time when 92 percent of companies are investing in data initiatives, which might prove unnecessary.

About the study

For the research, 122 managers in digital, advertising, publishing and software companies were asked about how they decided to allocate resources to new projects.

Among the ways they reported making decisions were:

  1. Majority: making the choice that most people wanted.
  2. Experience: going with the option that the most experienced individual preferred.
  3. Tallying: choosing the option with the most positive points.

The results showed that managers often relied on a ‘tallying’ approach more than other methods.

More analysis did not provide much of a boost to accuracy in decision-making and took considerably longer, the results showed.

Using instinct and rules of thumb, like tallying positive points, was just as accurate as more data analysis.

Dr Oguz A. Acar, study co-author, said:

“This research shows that data-driven decision-making is not the panacea in all situations and may not result in increased accuracy when facing uncertainty.

Under extreme uncertainty, managers, particularly those with more experience, should trust the expertise and instincts that have propelled them to such a position.

The nous developed over years as a leader can be a more effective than an analytical tool which, in situations of extreme uncertainty, could act as a hindrance rather than a driver of success.”

The study was published in the journal Psychology & Marketing (West et al., 2021).

The Confirmation Bias: Definition And Examples

The definition of the confirmation bias in psychology is that people search for information that confirms their view of the world and ignore what doesn’t fit.

The definition of the confirmation bias in psychology is that people search for information that confirms their view of the world and ignore what doesn’t fit.

The confirmation bias is the fact that people search for information that confirms their view of the world and ignore what doesn’t fit.

In an uncertain world, people love to be right because it helps them make sense of things.

Indeed, some psychologists think it’s akin to a basic drive.

What is the confirmation bias?

One of the ways they strive to be correct is by looking for evidence that confirms they are correct, sometimes with depressing or comic results:

  • A woman hires a worker that turns out to be incompetent. She doesn’t notice that everyone else is doing his work for him because she is so impressed that he shows up every day, right on time.
  • A sports fan who believes his team is the best only seems to remember the matches they won and none of the embarrassing defeats to inferior opponents.
  • A man who loves the country life, but has to move to the city for a new job, ignores the flight-path he lives under and noisy-neighbours-from-hell and tells you how much he enjoys the farmer’s market and tending his window box.

We do it automatically, usually without realising.

We do it partly because it’s easier to see where new pieces fit into the picture-puzzle we are working on, rather than imagining a new picture.

It also helps shore up our vision of ourselves as accurate, right-thinking, consistent people who know what’s what.

Psychologists call it the confirmation bias and it creeps into all sorts of areas of our lives.

Here are a few examples of the confirmation bias:

 1. Confirmation bias in self-image

“Hey, you look great, have you done something different with your hair?”

Who doesn’t like a compliment? No one. It doesn’t even have to be sincerely delivered, I’ll take it. But what about…

“Hey, you’re a real slime-ball, you know that?”

Who likes insults? Well, we don’t exactly like them but—believe it or not—sometimes we seek them out if they confirm our view of ourselves.

In a study that examined this, people actually sought out information confirming their own view that they were—not exactly slime-balls—but lazy, or slow-witted or not very athletic (Swann et al., 1989).

And this isn’t some kind of self-hating thing; in this study even people with high self-esteem sought out information that confirmed their own negative self-views.

It seems we like to be right, even at a cost to our self-image.

2. Finance example

A study of online stock market investors has looked at how they gathered information about a prospective stock (Park et al., 2010).

The researchers found the confirmation bias writ large.

Investors mostly looked for information that confirmed their hunch about a particular stock.

Those people who displayed the strongest confirmation bias were the most over-confident investors and consequently made the least money.

It seems we like to be right, even if it costs us money.

3. Politics example

People see what they want to see in politics all the time.

The most ironic example is in satire.

Often satire uses sarcasm to make its point: TV satirist Stephen Colbert frequently says the opposite of what he really thinks to make his point (amongst comedians I believe these are called ‘jokes’).

Except the irony is that one study has shown that people who don’t agree with Colbert don’t get that he’s being sarcastic, they think he really means it (LaMarre, 2009).

The beauty is that both liberals and conservatives get what they want: their viewpoints confirmed.

It seems we like to be right, even if it means not getting the joke.

4. Healthcare examples

Despite what many nurses believe, the full moon is NOT linked to busier hospital emergency rooms or more births (Margot, 2015).

The belief that there might be a link is likely down to the confirmation bias.

Despite the belief being remarkably common in hospitals, the study, published in the journal Nursing Research, found no evidence for it.

Similarly there’s NO evidence that the moon has any influence on:

  • automobile accidents,
  • hospital admissions,
  • surgery outcomes,
  • cancer survival rates,
  • menstruation,
  • births,
  • birth complications,
  • depression,
  • violent behaviour,
  • or even criminal activity.

Blame it on the confirmation bias

Over the years the confirmation bias has picked up the blame for all sorts of dodgy beliefs. Here are a few:

  • People are prejudiced (partly) because they only notice facts which fit with their preconceived notions about other nations or ethnicities.
  • People believe weird stuff about flying saucers, the JFK assassination, astrology, Egyptian pyramids and the moon landings because they only look for confirmation not dis-confirmation.
  • In the early nineteenth century doctors treated any old disease with blood-letting. Their patients sometimes got better so doctors—who conveniently ignored all the people who died—figured it must be doing something. In fact for many ailments some people will always get better on their own without any treatment at all.

Fight the bias

The way to fight the confirmation bias is simple to state but hard to put into practice.

You have to try and think up and test out alternative hypothesis.

Sounds easy, but it’s not in our nature.

It’s no fun thinking about why we might be misguided or have been misinformed. It takes a bit of effort.

It’s distasteful reading a book which challenges our political beliefs, or considering criticisms of our favourite film or, even, accepting how different people choose to live their lives.

Trying to be just a little bit more open is part of the challenge that the confirmation bias sets us.

Can we entertain those doubts for just a little longer?

Can we even let the facts sway us and perform that most fantastical of feats: changing our minds?

.

Hindsight Bias: What It Is And How To Avoid It

Hindsight bias can stop us learning from our mistakes and warp our predictions about future events.

Hindsight bias can stop us learning from our mistakes and warp our predictions about future events.

Hindsight bias is the common bias for people to assume that events could only have turned out the way they did.

The hindsight bias is sometimes known as the knew-it-all-along-phenomenon or creeping determinism.

Hindsight bias can distort memories, make people overconfident and change their predictions about future events.

What is hindsight bias?

The hindsight bias is our tendency towards thinking that things must have turned out the way they actually have.

Research carried out on professionals and lay people alike has confirmed the finding.

Time and again, the outcomes of medical diagnoses, legal decisions, elections and sporting events seem more likely after the answer is known.

We display this bias across many different areas of life.

The things that happen to us seem more like they were meant to happen.

This is partly because of our drive to make sense of the world; it’s comforting to feel we can predict what is happening to us and why.

Example of hindsight bias

An example of the hindsight bias from the world of business is a good way to understand it.

Going into business for yourself is scary.

Despite all the potential rewards, compared with getting a safe job with a big firm, being an entrepreneur means accepting huge risks.

All entrepreneurs know that there are no guarantees and that new businesses fail at a frighteningly high rate.

Still many manage to convince themselves that their venture will be different.

As you might expect, as a group entrepreneurs are remarkably optimistic about their chances of succeeding (otherwise why bother?).

One study asked 705 entrepreneurs who were about to start up a new business how they estimated their chances of success (Casser & Craig, 2009).

When the researchers got back to them a while later about 40 percent had quit their new business.

This 40 percent were then asked: what did you think your chances of success were before you started?

The first time they estimated their chances of success, before their business failed, they guessed, on average, 77.3 percent.

Afterwards they recalled this figure to be 58.8 percent.

In other words the failure of their business had made them revise their original estimate downwards.

With hindsight, then, the actual outcome had become more predictable.

Factors affecting the bias

The example of entrepreneurs nicely demonstrates the hindsight bias.

Under some circumstances, the hindsight bias is particularly strong:

  1. The impression of inevitability. The hindsight bias is stronger when you can easily identify a possible cause of the event. For example, your bag was stolen because you’re a tourist.
  2. The impression of foreseeability. The hindsight bias is stronger when you are you less surprised by what happened.

Explanation of the bias

The hindsight bias occurs because we revise our estimation of an event’s probability after the fact.

When you know you team won, it seems inevitable.

People naturally look for information that confirms their view of the world — we all want to be right.

Our memories aid us in this endeavour of proving ourselves right.

The bias can make people overconfident.

How to avoid the hindsight bias

The hindsight bias can be a problem when it stops us learning from our mistakes.

If the entrepreneurs knew how biased their estimates of success were, would they have done things differently?

If trainee doctors think a diagnosis was obvious all along, how will they learn to consider alternatives?

So psychologists have looked at ways in which we can correct for the hindsight bias.

The main one is forcing people to justify their judgements and think about alternative ways in which things could have turned out.

This normally makes people see that things could easily have turned out differently.

Of course, now you know about the hindsight bias, and how it can be corrected, it seems pretty obvious, doesn’t it?

.

Time Perception: Why Time Can Feel So Slow

Time can feel so slow because our perception is warped by life-threatening situations, eye movements, tiredness, hypnosis, age, the emotions and more…

Time can feel so slow because our perception is warped by life-threatening situations, eye movements, tiredness, hypnosis, age, the emotions and more…

The mind does funny things to our time perception.

Just ask French cave expert Michel Siffre.

In 1962 Siffre went to live in a cave that was completely isolated from mechanical clocks and natural light.

He soon began to experience a huge change in his perception of time.

When he tried to measure out two minutes by counting up to 120 at one-second intervals, it took him 5 minutes.

After emerging from the cave he guessed the trip had lasted 34 days.

He’d actually been down there for 59 days.

His perception of time was rapidly changing.

From an outside perspective he was slowing down, but the psychological experience for Siffre was that time was speeding up.

But you don’t have to hide out in a cave for a couple of months to warp time, it happens to us all the time.

Our experience of time is flexible; it depends on attention, motivation, the emotions and more.

1. Life-threatening situations

People often report that time seems to slow down in life-threatening situations, like skydiving.

But are we really processing more information in these seconds when time seems to stretch?

Is it like slow-motion cameras in sports which can actually see more details of the high-speed action?

To test this, Stetson et al. (2007) had people staring at a special chronometer while free-falling 50 metres into a net.

What they found was that time resolution doesn’t increase: we’re not able to distinguish shorter periods of time when in danger.

What happens is we remember the time as longer because we record more of the experience.

Life-threatening experiences make us really pay attention but we don’t gain superhuman powers of time perception.

2. Time doesn’t fly when you’re having fun

We’ve all experienced the fact that time seems to fly when we’re having fun.

Or does it?

What about when you’re listening to a fantastic uplifting piece of music?

Does time seem to fly by, or conversely, does it seem to slow down?

When this was tested by Kellaris (1992), they found that when listeners enjoyed the music more, time seemed to slow down.

This may be because when we enjoy music we listen more carefully, getting lost in it.

Greater attention leads to perception of a longer interval of time.

The same thing happens when you have a really good, exciting day out.

At the end of the day it can feel like you ate breakfast a lifetime ago.

You enjoyed yourself enormously and yet time has stretched out.

The fact that we intuitively believe time flies when we’re having fun may have more to do with how time seems to slow when we’re not having fun.

Boredom draws our attention to the passage of time which gives us the feeling that it’s slowing down.

Or—prepare yourself for a 180 degree about-face—it could all be the other way around.

Perhaps you’re having fun when time flies. In other words, we assume we’ve been enjoying ourselves when we notice that time has passed quickly.

There’s evidence for this in an experiment by Sackett et al. (2010).

Participants doing a boring task were tricked into thinking it had lasted half as long as it really had.

They thought it was more enjoyable than those who had been doing exactly the same task but who hadn’t been tricked about how much time had passed.

Ultimately it may come down to how much you believe that time flies when you’re having fun.

Sackett and colleagues tested this idea as well and found it was true.

In their experiments, people who believed more strongly in the idea that time flies when you’re having fun were more likely to believe they were having fun when time flew.

So, the whole thing could partly be a self-fulfilling prophecy.

3. The stopped clock illusion

The stopped clock illusion is a weird effect that you may have experienced.

It happens when you look at an analogue watch and the second-hand seems to freeze for longer than a second before moving on.

I always thought this was because I just happened to look at it right at the start of the second, but this is actually an illusion.

What is happening is that when your eyes move from one point to another (a saccade), your perception of time stretches slightly (Yarrow et al., 2001).

Weirdly, it stretches backwards.

So, your brain tells you that you’ve been looking at the watch for slightly longer than you really have.

Hence the illusion that the second-hand is frozen for more than a second.

This happens every time our eyes move from one fixation point to the next, it’s just that we only notice it when looking at a watch.

One explanation is that our brains are filling in the gap while our eyes move from looking at one thing to the next.

4. Too tired to tell the time

When things happen very close together in time, our brains fuse them together into a single snapshot of the present.

For vision the shortest interval we can perceive is about 80 milliseconds.

If two things happen closer together than that then we experience them as simultaneous.

The shortest possible gap in time we can distinguish across modalities (say visual and auditory) is between 20 and 60 milliseconds (Fink et al., 2006).

That’s as little as a fiftieth of a second.

When we’re tired, though, our perception of time goes awry and we find it more difficult to distinguish between short spaces of time.

This fact can be used to measure whether people are too tired to fly a plane, drive a truck or be a doctor.

Indeed just such simple hand-held devices that quickly assess your tiredness are already being developed (Eagleman, 2009).

5. Self-regulation stretches time perception

The effort of trying to either suppress or enhance our emotional reactions seems to change our perception of time.

Psychologists have found that when people are trying to regulate their emotions, time seems to drag on.

Vohs and Schmeichel (2003) had participants watch an 11 minute clip from the film Terms of Endearment.

Some participants were asked to remain emotionally neutral while watching the clip and others were told to act naturally.

Those who tried to suppress their emotions estimated the clip had lasted longer than it really had.

6. Altered states of consciousness

People report all sorts of weird experiences with time when taking drugs like psilocybin, peyote or LSD.

Time can seem to speed up, slow down, go backwards, or even stop.

But you don’t need drugs to enter an altered state of consciousness, hypnosis will do the trick.

People generally seem to underestimate the time that they’ve been under hypnosis.

One study found this figure was around 40 percent (Bowers & Brenneman, 1979).

7. Does time perception change with age?

People often say the years pass more quickly as they get older.

While youthful summers seemed to stretch on into infinity, the summers of your later years zip by in the blink of an eye.

A common explanation for this is that everything is new when we are young so we pay more attention; consequently it feels like time perception expands.

With age, though, new experiences diminish and it tends to be more of the same, so time seems to pass more quickly.

Whether or not this is true, there is some psychological evidence that time passes quicker for older people.

One study has found that people in their 20s are pretty accurate at guessing an interval of 3 minutes, but people in their 60s systematically overestimate it, suggesting time is passing about 20 percent more quickly for them (Mangan & Bolinsky, 1997).

8. The emotional experience of time perception

The emotions we feel in the moment directly affect our perception of time.

Negative emotions in particular seem to bring time to people’s attention and so make it seem longer.

Research on anxious cancer patients, those with depression and boredom-prone individuals suggests time perception stretches out for them (reported in Wittmann, 2009).

Just like life-threatening situations, negative emotions can concentrate our attention on the passage of time and so make it seem longer than it really is.

This effect may be made worse by our efforts to regulate these negative emotions (see number 5), which also has the effect of stretching time.

9. Body temperature changes time perception

If you’ve ever had a fever then you’ll know that body temperature can have strange effects on time perception.

Experiments have found that when body temperature is raised our perception of time speeds up (Wearden & Pento-Voak, 1995).

Conversely when we are cooled down, our sense of time also slows down.

10. What’s your tempo?

Setting aside emotions, age, drugs and all the rest, our perception of time is also affected by who we are.

People seem to operate to different beats; we’ve all met people who work at a much slower or faster pace than we do.

Psychologists have found that people who are impulsive and oriented towards the present tend to find that time moves faster for them than others (from O’Brien et al., 2011).

There’s little research on this but it’s likely that each of us has our own personal tempo.

Research has found that when different people listen to metronomes the number of beats per minute they describe as comfortable ranges from as slow as 40 bpm up to a high of 200 bpm (Kir-Stimon, 1977).

This is a large range and may help to explain why some people seem to operate at such a different pace to ourselves.

Time perception is relative

The last words on time come from two great thinkers; first Albert Einstein:

“Put your hand on a hot stove for a minute, and it seems like an hour.

Sit with a pretty girl for an hour, and it seems like a minute.

That’s relativity.”

And finally, Douglas Adams, author of The Hitchhiker’s Guide to the Galaxy and the Dirk Gently books:

“Time is an illusion.

Lunchtime doubly so.”

.

Precrastination: Why People Complete Tasks Early When There’s No Need

Precrastinators may answer an email too quickly or submit a report too early with too little information.

Precrastinators may answer an email too quickly or submit a report too early with too little information.

Keep reading with a Membership

• Read members-only articles
• Adverts removed
• Cancel at any time
• 14 day money-back guarantee for new members

Allowing The Mind To Wander Is More Pleasant Than We Predict (M)

Most people dislike spending time alone thinking and predict it will be unpleasant.

Most people dislike spending time alone thinking and predict it will be unpleasant.

Keep reading with a Membership

• Read members-only articles
• Adverts removed
• Cancel at any time
• 14 day money-back guarantee for new members

People Who Write Well Do This One Simple Thing

The method could benefit people using computers, pen-and-paper or even speech-to-text.

The method could benefit people using computers, pen-and-paper or even speech-to-text.

Forcing yourself to type slower could improve the quality of your writing, a study finds.

Participants in the study who typed with only one hand produced higher quality essays, researchers found.

Mr Srdan Medimorec, the study’s lead author, said:

“Typing can be too fluent or too fast, and can actually impair the writing process.

It seems that what we write is a product of the interactions between our thoughts and the tools we use to express them.”

People who type quickly may use the first word that comes to hand.

Slowing down allows the mind more time to find the right word.

This could be why forcing yourself to slow down a little improves the sophistication of vocabulary used.

Professor Evan F. Risko, who co-authored the study, said:

“This is the first study to show that when you interfere with people’s typing, their writing can get better.

We’re not saying that students should write their term papers with one hand, but our results show that going fast can have its drawbacks.

This is important to consider as writing tools continue to emerge that let us get our thoughts onto the proverbial page faster and faster.”

Slowing down your writing could help writing quality no matter what input method is used, the authors think.

The same trick could benefit people using pen-and-paper or even speech-to-text.

Slowing down too much, though, can be detrimental.

When people slow to below the rate of normal handwriting, their quality gets worse, previous research suggests.

The study was published in the British Journal of Psychology (Medimorec & Risko, 2016).

Get free email updates

Join the free PsyBlog mailing list. No spam, ever.